Illusions and visual special effects – explanations and tutorials

Optical Illusions

Archives Posts

Doggendorff and Moggendorff Illusions

December 8th, 2009 by david

Moggendorff and Doggendorff versions of the Poggendorff illusion

Here are a couple more variants of the Poggendorff illusion (mog, or moggy, by the way, is a term of endearment for a cat in UK English, but I’m not sure it’ll be familiar if your background is in American English). The symmetry axes of the dog and cat heads are objectively aligned, but to my eye appear displaced in much the way that the (objectively aligned) test line appears to be in classic versions of the illusion (as in pale blue, to the left).

I’ve added the blobs to the dog version, and the pigeons to the cat figure, because I have the impression that they make the illusion a little stronger. However, I haven’t tested that experimentally with these figures.  It’s also interesting to try deleting the images progressively, to see how much can be deleted before the illusion vanishes. Maybe there are conventional Poggendorff figure elements embedded in these figures in a way I haven’t realised.

For example, it’s well established that the illusion can arise when the usual line elements are reduced just to dots, (the dot version that might apply here is Stanley Coren’s – scroll down that link to view it). It would be possible to selectively erase the figures here until just dots were left. But reduced to dots the illusion is very weak, and here it looks quite robust to me.

I think it is the symmetry axes that are taking the place of the usual test lines here.  For me, that makes it that much more likely that the illusion arises because of two dimensional pattern elements.  (However, many specialists don’t agree, and attribute the illusion to attempts by the brain to interpret Poggendorff figures as arrays of lines in depth).

I have a special interest in this illusion, and you’ll find stacks more on it by clicking on the Poggendorff illusion category, in the categories list to the right.  I have ideas about what I think might be going on – but actually, I don’t rate them all that highly. Sometimes in science, when a problem resists progress for a very long time, (over a century in the case of this illusion), so that there are all sorts of ingenious competing explanations, it’s a sign that something is going on that nobody’s even begun to imagine.  I think that could well be the case with Poggendorff.

Archives Posts

Meet Humbaba

December 7th, 2009 by david

Humbaba

This is one of the oldest ambiguous images I know. It’s a small clay mask from Mesapotamia, (in modern day Iraq), made about 3750 years ago. It’s the face of the giant Humbaba, but as he might have appeared to a soothsayer, looking into the writhing entrails of a sacrificed animal for purposes of divination. If the face of Humbaba appeared in the entrails, in the way we sometimes see a face in clouds, it was a sign of revolution on the way. This evocation of the experience is in the British Museum, and they have a web page about it. (They even know the soothsayer who made it …).

I’ve written in earlier posts about the way that artists often seem to use perceptual puzzles as a starting point for aesthetic and emotional effects in artworks. This is a particularly fascinating example. It’s a work of art, but it’s also a record of emotional effect arising out of a perceptual puzzle, an ambiguous image, in a quite different kind of activity – divination. If I’ve got it right, quite a lot of fortune telling starts with ambiguous visual discoveries like this, when peering into tea-leaves, or crystal balls. I wonder how deep the common roots of aesthetics and shamanistic experiences go.

One route you can trace is through the entrails. You can’t quite be sure in this image, but when you look at the real thing, so you can look round the edges, the face is made up of one continuous entrail, coiling to and fro. If you can get to the British Museum, it’s in a case in their new Mesapotamia gallery, but you may have to hunt around, it’s not big.

Archives Posts

Pinna’s Intertwining Illusion

December 5th, 2009 by david

Pinna's intersecting spirals illusion

This is a brilliant illusion discovered by Baingio Pinna of the University of Sassari in Italy.  The circles appear to spiral and intersect, but are in fact an orderly set of concentric circles. The illusion is due to the way the orientation of the squares alternates from circle to circle, and that contrast alternates from square to square within each circle. The illusion is related to the movement illusions of Akiyoshi Kitaoka and to twisted cord illusions.

What’s going on is suggested by this next version, with the edges enhanced, plus a bit of blurring.

Filtered version of Pinna's intersecting spirals illusion

This image approximates (with false colour) the data transmitted within the brain once the image has been filtered by cell systems early in the visual pathway, including centre-surround cell assemblies (a bit technical, that link). The role of these is to enhance edges, so that bright edges are now emphasised by dark  fringes and vice versa. Note that between the little stacks of alternating light and dark fringes, along the line of the circles, the dark fringes of bright squares align with the dark edges of adjacent squares and vice versa. The scale and spacing of the squares is just right to get that alignment, and as a result the effect enhances the inward turning, spiralling effect due to the orientation of the squares. The fringes combine to give an effect a little like interfering waves. The illusion seems to be bamboozling processes that are usually superbly effective at filtering out the key information about edges and their orientation in the visual field.

However, showing that centre-surround cell outputs could be enhancing the inward turning character of the lines forming the large circles doesn’t explain why the brain integrates the local effects into the perception that the large circles as a whole are spiralling inwards. I guess that’s because, to a much greater extent than we realise, we infer global configurations from what we see just in the central, foveal area of the field of view. That also seems to be the case with impossible 3 dimensional shapes, as in the impossible tribar.